Monday, August 5, 2013

Whenever I'm Alone With You (When No One's Around)

YOUR MIND IS WIRED (THRU GENES) TO BELIEVE IN NONSENSE (GOD AND RELIGIOUS BELIEFS). UNDERSTAND WHY BELOW.

GO ABOUT 3 QUARTERS DOWN THIS POST AND READ THE PARAGRAPHS ABOUT RECIPROCAL ALTRUISM, CAUSAL AGENTS, OUR AGENT-DETECTION DEVICE, AND OUR ERROR PRONE MEMORY. THEN TRY TO UNDERSTAND HOW THESE MENTAL MECHANISMS (ADAPTATIONS) LED TO THE EVOLUTION OF BELIEF IN GOD AND WHY THEY CONTINUE TO LED YOU TO BELIEVE IN GOD TODAY. (YOU ARE ALL FUCKIN' DELUDED AND LIVING IN MAKE BELIEVE WORLD BECAUSE YOUR MIND IS PREDISPOSED TO DELUDE YOU AND MAKE YOU BELIEVE IN MAKE BELIEVE WORLD.)


It is also possible that religious belief - and particularly, faith in one or a small number of very powerful dieties - derives from a this-worldly primate tendency to worshipfully obey a dominant leader. Jay Glass has made the interesting argument that "In the original state of nature, for both animals and humans, loyalty to a Supreme Being (aka dominant male, king, warlord, etc.) offered protection from enemies and provided the necessities to sustain life. Those that did not put their faith and trust in a god-like figure did not survive to produce the next generation. The jewel in Glass's argument is his reworking of the 23rd Psalm, as it might describe members of a chimpanzee troop speaking of the dominant male:


"HE FILLS MY CUP" - #TRUTH
Belief In God's Power (His Omniscience, His Omnipotence, His Omnipresence, Etc.) Is Based In The Genetically Inherited Tendency For Humans To Subordinate Themselves And Place Their Faith, Trust, And Lives In The Hand Of A Dominant Leader (The Alpha Male Of The Group). We Evolved This Tendency Over Millions Of Years And Have Now Redirected It To The Concept Of God (God Is The Ultimate Alpha Male).
...it seems likely that insofar as a primate troop member "worships" his or her leader, at least the existence of that leader is undeniable, along with (in most cases) the negative consequences of deviation. On the other hand, to my knowledge, God seems on balance less likely to strike down disbelievers than a dominant animal is to punish would-be rebels. Disbelief in God thus seems less costly (at least in the short term) than is failure to honor and obey one's flesh-and-blood leader. Yet, as we  shall soon consider, it is also possible that religion has established and maintained itself precisely by exacting temporal punishment against apostates, which not only harkens back to Richard Dawkins's hypothesis of religious belief as parasitic meme but also provides a potential mechanism whereby religion could conceivably be selected for at the level of groups.

There seems little doubt, in any event, that numerous payoffs can be derived by followers of religion no less than those following a dominant, secular leader, who participate in a group whose shared followership results in greater coherence and thus enhanced biological and social success.

Homo Mysterious: Evolutionary Puzzles of Human Nature. Barash, p. 248-250.

Header essay v1 gorillas  455168123
ALPHA 2 OMEGA

http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s40806-015-0039-z
Man Created God in His Image: A Review of Hector Garcia, Alpha God: The Psychology of Religious Violence and Oppression

The evolutionary origin of religions theorizes about the emergence of religious behavior during the course of human evolution.

Humanity's closest living relatives are common chimpanzees and bonobos. These primates share a common ancestor with humans who lived between four and six million years ago. It is for this reason that chimpanzees and bonobos are viewed as the best available surrogate for this common ancestor. Barbara King argues that while non-human primates are not religious, they do exhibit some traits that would have been necessary for the evolution of religion. These traits include high intelligence, a capacity for symbolic communication, a sense of social norms, realization of "self" and a concept of continuity.[1][2][3] There is inconclusive evidence that Homo neanderthalensis may have buried their dead which is evidence of the use of ritual. The use of burial rituals is evidence of religious activity, but there is no other evidence that religion existed in human culture before humans reached behavioral modernity.[4]

Marc Bekoff, Professor Emeritus of Ecology and Evolutionary Biology at the University of Colorado, Boulder, argues that many species grieve death and loss.[5]

There is general agreement among cognitive scientists that religion is an outgrowth of brain architecture that evolved early in human history. However, there is disagreement on the exact mechanisms that drove the evolution of the religious mind. The two main schools of thought hold that either religion evolved due to natural selection and has selective advantage, or that religion is an evolutionary byproduct of other mental adaptations.[18] Stephen Jay Gould, for example, believed that religion was an exaptation or a spandrel, in other words that religion evolved as byproduct of psychological mechanisms that evolved for other reasons.[19][20][21]

Such mechanisms may include the ability to infer the presence of organisms that might do harm (agent detection), the ability to come up with causal narratives for natural events (etiology), and the ability to recognize that other people have minds of their own with their own beliefs, desires and intentions (theory of mind). These three adaptations (among others) allow human beings to imagine purposeful agents behind many observations that could not readily be explained otherwise, e.g. thunder, lightning, movement of planets, complexity of life, etc.[22] The emergence of collective religious belief identified the agents as deities that standardized the explanation.

Some scholars have suggested that religion is genetically "hardwired" into the human condition. One controversial hypothesis, the God gene hypothesis, states that some variants of a specific gene, the VMAT2 gene, predispose to spirituality.[23]

Another view is based on the concept of the triune brain: the reptilian brain, the limbic system, and the neocortex, proposed by Paul D. MacLean. Collective religious belief draws upon the emotions of love, fear, and gregariousness and is deeply embedded in the limbic system through sociobiological conditioning and social sanction. Individual religious belief utilizes reason based in the neocortex and often varies from collective religion. The limbic system is much older in evolutionary terms than the neocortex and is, therefore, stronger than it much in the same way as the reptilian is stronger than both the limbic system and the neocortex. Reason is pre-empted by emotional drives. The religious feeling in a congregation is emotionally different from individual spirituality even though the congregation is composed of individuals. Belonging to a collective religion is culturally more important than individual spirituality though the two often go hand in hand. This is one of the reasons why religious debates are likely to be inconclusive.

Yet another view is that the behaviour of people who participate in a religion makes them feel better and this improves their fitness, so that there is a genetic selection in favor of people who are willing to believe in religion. Specifically, rituals, beliefs, and the social contact typical of religious groups may serve to calm the mind (for example by reducing ambiguity and the uncertainty due to complexity) and allow it to function better when under stress.[24] This would allow religion to be used as a powerful survival mechanism, particularly in facilitating the evolution of hierarchies of warriors, which if true, may be why many modern religions tend to promote fertility and kinship.


http://jamesvanslyke.blogspot.com/2008/09/welcome-to-blog-apu-students.html
GO 3 QUARTERS OF THE WAY DOWN THIS PAGE TO WHERE IT SAYS Psychology of Religion AND CLICK ON Primates and Religion OR YOU CAN JUST CLICK ON Primates and Religion


A fundamental characteristic of all primates, the need for belongingness is most elaborated in the African apes, our closest living relatives. Though we did not descend from chimpanzees or gorillas, we share with them a common ancestor. The everyday social behavior of this apelike ancestor laid a foundation for the evolution of religion that was to come much later, a foundation that can be reconstructed from knowledge of what today’s apes do.
Drawing on my own years of up-close-and-personal encounters with chimpanzees and gorillas, I discuss in Chapter 2 the early precursors to religion—empathy, meaning-making, rule-following, and imagination— and how these relate to the issue of ape consciousness. I am convinced that apes are highly sensitive and tuned in to one another starting in infancy, when a baby begins to negotiate with its mother about its needs. More than most other mammals, ape infants are born into a highly social world, a web of emotional interactions among relatives and other social partners. Research on animals like dolphins and elephants may someday challenge this conclusion, but it seems clear at least that the way two apes respond to each other sensitively and contingently is of different quality than what happens when two wolves, say, or two domestic cats, circle each other and adjust to each other’s snarls, or lunges, in a well-honed, highly instinctual dance. It even seems different from the learned behaviors of other primates, like monkeys. The apes’ finely tuned responses to each other are rooted in belongingness, in the emotionality toward others that stems from their being so keenly dependent on their mothers and other relatives from birth onward.
Second, profound changes in emotional relating occurred as our human ancestors’ lives diverged from those of the apelike ancestors. In Chapters 3 through 6, I focus on the origins of the human religious imagination in the span of time bounded, on the one end, by the divergence of hominids (human ancestors) from the ape lineage about 6 million or 7 million years ago, and on the other by the beginning of farming and settled communities around 10,000 years ago. Admittedly, we can glean almost nothing concrete about emotional connectedness as far back as 7 million years (though we can continue to use modern day apes as models, and speculate in useful ways).After 3 million years ago, the record of material culture—fossilized artifacts and other concrete products of hominid behavior—begins. At that point, tangible clues help us assess the changes that take place in empathy, meaning making, rule-following, imagination, and consciousness, and, indeed, in the pattern of nurturing and caring that lays the foundation for all of these.
After all, it is not the stones and bones, the technology and art, that deserve top billing in our prehistory; it is material culture’s emotional backstory that does. Throughout the millennia, hominid mothers nurtured their children; siblings played with each other and with their friends; adults shifted alliances, supporting first this friend, then another, against a rival. The emotional dependency of ape infants on their mothers and other relatives only deepened and lengthened as the human lineage began to evolve, a fact with cascading consequences for the hominids’ whole lives.
The archaeologist Steven Mithen rescues Neandertals, for instance, from the caveman-dragging-cavewoman-by-the-hair stereotype by acknowledging this rich inner life; he writes of “intensely emotional beings: happy Neanderthals, sad Neanderthals, angry Neanderthals, disgusted Neanderthals, envious Neanderthals, guilty Neanderthals, grief-stricken Neanderthals, and Neanderthals in love.”1 While I embrace Mithen’s sensibility, I would have put the statement a bit differently: “Neandertals making each other happy, Neandertals making each other sad . . .” Emotions, before, after, and during the Neandertal period, are created when individuals act together and make meaning together, starting in infancy. The excitement in understanding human evolution is centered in tracing this mutual creativity and meaning making, indeed in tracing the evolution of belongingness.
Third, the hominid need for belongingness rippled out, eventually expanding into a wholly new realm. In tandem with, and in part driven by, changes in the natural environment, in the hominid brain, and most important, in caregiving practices, something new emerged that went beyond empathy, rule-following, and imagination within the family and immediate group, and that went beyond consciousness expressed through action and meaning-making in the here and now. As I explain in Chapters 6 and 7, language and culture became more complex as symbols and ritual practices began to play a more central role in how hominids made sense of their world. An earthly need for belongingness led to the human religious imagination and thus to the otherworldly realm of relating with God, gods, and spirits.
From the building blocks we find in apelike ancestors emerged the soulful need to pray to gods, to praise God with hymns, to shake in terror before the power of invisible spirits, to fear for one’s life at the hands of the unknown or to feel bathed in all-enveloping love from the heavens. To express in straightforward language the profound depth of this human emotional connection to the sacred is a challenge. The inaccessibility to language of the sacred experience mirrors what Martin Buber writes about when he describes human relating with God: it “is wrapped in a cloud but reveals itself, it lacks but creates language. We hear no You and yet we feel addressed; we answer—creating, thinking, acting: with our being we speak the basic word, unable to say You with our mouth.”2
Why would an anthropologist who studies apes be interested in religion? 
I think religion is all about emotional engagement and social action. And we can get a whole new read on the evolutionary history of religion by asking the kinds of questions that we ask of language and culture. We can see that way back in our past — literally, millions of years ago — some practices are visible in the archaeological record that reflect the deepest roots of religion. And apes today are pretty good stand-ins for those very early human ancestors. So when I go to the National Zoo in Washington, or spend time in Kenya looking at monkeys, what I see is very social. It’s about emotional connection that’s at the very ancient roots of religion. 

So you’re not saying that the great apes you study are religious — or have spiritual lives — but they show behaviors that are required if you’re going to develop religion.
That’s right. I’m not suggesting that apes are religious. In fact, I have to say that, because Jane Goodall, who is such a renowned and loved figure for her chimpanzee studies, has said very provocatively that chimpanzees may have an incipient sense of religious awe. For example, when she comes upon them looking at a waterfall — something in nature that is amazing — they’re riveted. She’s wondering what’s going through their minds and if they may be spiritual in some sense. That’s a fascinating idea, but that’s not my approach. I don’t look for things in apes that are religious. I look at how their behavior relates to the very foundation of what later became religion. For me, the question turns on how I understand religion. I want to be very careful to differentiate between what we think about religion today and how it evolved. I’m really talking about the earliest origins of religion, which was a social and emotional process. 

So you’re not talking about a set of beliefs? I think that’s how most people think about religion.
I’m not talking about a set of beliefs. When I think about religion, what comes to mind are personal relationships with the supernatural, with God or with spirits, and compassionate action. Not necessarily books or texts that you read, but some sort of action in the world. This is coming from Karen Armstrong’s work, who has helped me let go of the idea that religion is about a bunch of things in our head that we have to feel and believe. So if I’m going to think about religion as compassionate action, how do you look for that in prehistory? That’s the real question that I face as an anthropologist. And the way I approach that is to look at the active expression of this emotional connection in something that I can identify as a spiritual realm.




When something appears in every known society, as religion does, the question of whether it is "in the genes" naturally arises. Did religion confer such benefits on our distant ancestors that genes favoring it spread by natural selection? There are scientists who believe the answer is yes - enough of them, in fact, to give rise to headlines like this one, in a Canadian newspaper: "'Search continues for God Gene.'"

Expect to see the headline again, for the search is unlikely to reach a successful conclusion. And that isn't just because, obviously, no single gene could undergird something as complex as religion. Things don't look good even for the more nuanced version of the "God gene" idea - that a whole bunch of genes were preserved by natural selection because they inclined people toward religion.

Oddly, this verdict - that religion isn't in any straightforward sense "in the genes" - emerges from evolutionary psychology, a field that has been known to emphasize genetic influences on thought and emotion. Though some evolutionary psychologists think that religion is a direct product of natural selection, many - and probably most - don't.

This doesn't mean religion isn't in any sense "natural," and it doesn't mean religion isn't in some sense "in the genes." Everything people do is in some sense in the genes. (Try doing something without using any genes.) What's more, we can trace religion to specific parts of human nature that are emphatically in the genes. It's just that those parts of human nature seem to have evolved for some reason other than to sustain religion.

The American psychologist William James, in his 1902 classic The Varieties of Religious Experience, captured the basic idea without referring to evolution: "There is religious fear, religious love, religious awe, religious joy, and so forth. But religious love is only man's natural emotion of love directed to a religious object; religious fear is only the ordinary fear of commerce, so to speak, the common quaking of the human breast, in so far as the notion of divine retribution may arouse it; religious awe is the same organic thrill which we feel in a forest at twilight, or in a mountain gorge; only this time it comes over us at the thought of our supernatural relations."

If you want to put James's basic point in the language of evolutionary biology, you have to drag in the concept of an "adaptation." An adaptation is a trait whose underlying genes spread through the gene pool by virtue of their giving rise to that trait. Love, for example, seems to be an adaptation. Love of offspring, by inspiring nurturance of those offspring, can help genes get into future generations; as a result, genes underlying parental love seem to have spread by virtue of  their conduciveness to love. You can similarly make arguments that awe and joy and fear - the other sentiments that James cites - were, in themselves, adaptations. (Fearing a big aggressive animal, or a big aggressive human being, could save your skin and thus save the genes underlying the fear.) But that doesn't mean religion is an adaptation, even though religion may involve love, awe, joy, and fear and thus involve the genes underlying these things. 

To shift back into less technical terminology: you might say that we were "designed" by natural selection to feel love and awe and joy and fear. (So long as you understand that "designed" is a metaphor; natural selection isn't like a human designer who consciously envisions the end product and then realizes it, but is rather a blind, dumb process of trial and error.) But to say that these emotions are a product of "design" isn't to say that when they're activated by religion they're working as "designed."

Similarly, humans were "designed" by natural selection to be able to run and were also "designed" to feel competitive spirit, but that doesn't mean they were "designed" to participate in track meets. Religion, like track, doesn't seem to be an "adaptation." Both seem to be what the paleontologist Stephen Jay Gould Called a "Spandrel" - a phenomenon supported by genes that had become part of the species by doing something other than supporting that phenomenon. A spandrel is an incidental by-product of the organic "design" process, whereas an adaptation is a direct product. Religion seems to be a spandrel.

And yet, you might say, religion does have the hallmarks of design. It is a complex, integrated system that seems to serve specific functions. For example, religions almost always handle some key "rites of passage" - getting married, getting buried, and so on - whose ritualized handling is probably good for the society. How do you explain the coherence and functionality of religion without appealing to a designer - or, at least, a "designer"?

You don't. But biological evolution isn't the only great "designer" at work on this planet. There is also cultural evolution: the selective transmission of "memes" - beliefs, habits, rituals, songs, technologies, theories, and so forth - from person to person. And one criterion that shapes cultural evolution is social utility; memes that are conducive to smooth functioning at the group level often have an advantage over memes that aren't. Cultural evolution is what gave us modern corporations, modern government, and modern religion.

For that matter, it gave us nonmodern religion. Wherever we look at "primitive" religion, we are looking at a religion that has been evolving culturally for a long time. Though observed hunter-gatherer religions give clues about what the average religion was like 12,000 years ago, before the invention of agriculture, none of them much resembles religion in its literally "primitive" phase, the time (whenever that was) when religious beliefs and practices emerged. Rather, what are called "primitive" religions are bodies of belief and practice that have been evolving - culturally - over tens or even hundreds of millennia. Generation after generation, human  minds have been accepting some beliefs, rejecting others, shaping and reshaping religion along the way.

So to explain the existence of "primitive" religion - or for that matter any any other kind of religion - we have to first understand what kinds of beliefs and practices the human mind is amendable to. What kinds of information does the mind naturally filter out, and what kinds naturally penetrate it? Before religion appeared and started evolving by cultural evolution, how had genetic evolution shaped the environment in which it would evolve - that is, the human brain?

To put the question another way: What kinds of beliefs was the human mind "designed" by natural selection to harbor? For starters, not true ones.

At least, not true ones per se. To the extent that accurate perceptions and comprehension of the world helped humanity's ancestors get genes into the next generation, then of course mental accuracy would be favored by natural selection. And usually mental accuracy is good for the survival and transmission of the genes. That's why we have excellent equipment for depth perception, for picking up human voices against background noise, and so on. Still, in situations where accurate perception and judgement impede survival and reproduction, you would expect natural selection to militate against accuracy.

In 1974, San Francisco newspaper heiress Patty Hearst was kidnapped by a radical group called the Symbionese Liberation Army, whose goals included "death to the fascist insect that preys upon the life of the people." After being kept in a closet for a while, she came to identify with her new peer group. Before long, she was enthusiastically helping them generate income, at one point brandishing a machine gun during a bank robbery. When left alone, with an opportunity to escape, she didn't take it.

She later described the experience: "I had virtually no free will until I was separated from them for about two weeks. And then it suddenly, you know, slowly began to dawn that they just weren't there anymore. I could actually think my own thoughts." Hearst didn't just accept her captors' "subjective" beliefs, such as ideology; she bought into their views about how the physical world works. One of her captors "didn't want me thinking about rescue because he thought that brain waves could be read or that, you know, they'd get a psychic in to find me. And I was even afraid of that."

Hearst's condition of coerced credulity is called the Stockholm syndrome, after a kidnapping in Sweden. But the term "syndrome" may be misleading in its suggestion of abnormality. Hearst's response to her circumstances was probably an example of human nature functioning properly; we seem to be "designed" by natural selection to be brainwashed.

Some people find this prospect a shocking affront to human autonomy, but they tend not to be evolutionary psychologists. In Darwinian terms, it makes sense that our species could contain genes encouraging blind credulity in at least some situations. If you are surrounded by a small group of people on whom your survival depends, rejecting the beliefs that are most important to them will not help you live long enough to get your genes into the next generation.

Confinement with a small group of people may sound so rare that natural selection would have little chance to take account of it, but it is in a sense the natural human condition. Humans evolved in small groups - twenty, forty, sixty people - from which emigration was often not a  viable option. Survival depended on social support: sharing food, sticking together during fights, and so on. To alienate your peers by stubbornly contesting their heartfelt beliefs would have lowered your chances of genetic proliferation.

Maybe that explains why you don't have to lock somebody in a closet to get a bit of the Stockholm syndrome. Religious cults just offer aimless teenagers a free bus ride to a free meal, and after the recruits have been surrounded by believers for a few days, they tend to warm up to the beliefs. And there doesn't have to be some powerful authority figure pushing the beliefs. In one famous social psychology experiment, subjects opined that two lines of manifestly different lengths were the same length, once a few of their "peers" (who in fact were confederates) voiced that opinion.


Cube Captivate Your Mind!: Patty Hurst! - Rubix Cube

Given this conformist bias in human nature, it's not surprising that people born into "primitive" religions - or any other religions - accept an elaborate belief system that outside observers find highly dubious. But the question remains: How did the elaborate belief system ever come to exist? Granted that people are inclined to accept their community's official edifice of belief and ritual (especially if no alternatives are on offer). But how did the edifice come to exist in the first place? How did religion get built from the ground up?

To answer this question we have to view cultural evolution at a fine-grain level. We have to think about individual units of culture - beliefs and practices, in this case - and how they spread. The biologist Richard Dawkins coined the term "meme" for units of culture, in part because it sounded a bit like "gene," and he wanted to stress some parallels between cultural and biological evolution. For example: just as genes are transmitted from body to body, down the generations, memes are transmitted from mind to mind. And just as newly minted genes "compete" for a place in the gene pool, newly minted memes "compete" for the finite space in the world's supply of human brains. In this constant struggle of meme against meme, what kinds of memes will have a "selective advantage"?

Newspapers are a good place to look for clues. Newspaper editors work hard to figure out what kinds of information people want, and to fill that demand. They are accomplished meme engineers, and thus students of human nature. One thing you'll notice about newspapers is that they have a bias toward good things and bad things. The headlines "Stock market rises by 5 percent" and Stock market drops by 5 percent" will get better play than the headline "Stock market does nothing in particular." Here religions, and certainly "primitive" religions, are like newspapers. In every hunter-gatherer society, religion is devoted largely to explaining why bad things happen and why good things happen - illness, recovery; famine, abundance; and so on.

There is also devotion to raising the ratio of good to bad. The Andaman Islanders, convinced that whistling at night attracts spirits whereas singing repels them, do more singing in the dark than whistling in the dark. People naturally try to exert control over their environment, and believing that they have such control naturally makes them feel good. So people's minds are open to ideas that promise to give them such control. This doesn't mean people uncritically embrace every such idea that comes their way. But it does mean that these ideas get their attention - and for a meme, that's the first step toward acceptance. While the Andaman  Island meme asserting that thunderstorms are divine punishment for melting beeswax was hardly guaranteed a place in the society's religion, it had a big head start over memes saying "Thunderstorms just happen - there's nothing you can do about it."

Another thing you'll notice in newspapers is that the strange and novel wins out over the ordinary and expected. Tuberculosis and the West Nile virus are both bad news, and in terms of the number of people killed, tuberculosis is the worse of the two. Yet the headline "Outbreak of deadly new virus puzzles experts" easily crowds out "Usual number of people expected to die of tuberculosis this year"...As journalism sages famously put it: "Dog bites man" is not a story; "Man bites dog" is a story.

It makes sense that human brains would naturally seize on strange, surprising things, since the predictable things have already been absorbed into the expectations that guide them through the world; news of the strange and surprising may signal that some amendment of our expectations is warranted. But one property of strange, surprising claims is that they're often untrue. So if they get preferred access to our brains, that gives falsehood a kind of advantage - if a fleeting advantage - over the truth. In the days after the terrorist attacks of September 11, 2001, one widely circulated story was that a man at the top of one of the twin towers had survived by sliding down the rubble as it formed. It was a story so incredible that it virtually compelled you to click the "forward" icon on your email - and a story so incredible that it wasn't true. It was an example of the famous dictum that a lie can get halfway around the world before the truth has a chance to get its boots on.

Of course, in the long run, the truth often does get its boots on, and people often welcome it upon its arrival. Indeed, if the attraction to surprising news weren't balance by an attraction to claims that survive subsequent scrutiny, the average human ancestor wouldn't have lived long enough to become a human ancestor. Imagine a local sage, 200,000 years ago, saying that eating a certain berry will let you live forever. Now imagine that the first two people who follow his advice drop dead. Genes that counseled continued faith in advice thus besieged by countervailing evidence would not long remain a part of the species, whereas genes that inclined the brain to take account of such evidence might. This natural human respect for evidence is the reason convincing someone that one plus one equals three, or that water flows uphill, takes real work.

But some kinds of beliefs are harder to test than those two. And hard-to-test beliefs could do well in the process of cultural evolution that gave birth to religion. Indeed, hunter-gatherer religious belief - like religious belief generally - consists largely of claims that resist falsification. The Haida, a people indigenous to the northwest coast of North America, when caught in a storm while out at sea, would try to appease the relevant authorities (killer whale deities) by pouring a cup of fresh water into the sea or putting some tobacco or deer tallow on the end of a paddle. Many people no doubt returned from sea to report that these measures had kept them from drowning. No one, presumably, ever reported that they had taken these measures but drowned anyway.

To be sure, some religious beliefs can be put to a clearer test. If the Andaman Islanders were right, and melting beeswax was a leading cause of thunderstorms, then a melting moratorium should cut down on the thunderstorms. But how can you be sure that, in the days preceding a thunderstorm, no one in your village melted a smidgen of wax - or engaged in some other thunder-inducing activity, such as making a loud noise while the cicadas were singing?

Such loopholes are found in modern religions, too. If you pray for someone to recover from illness, and they don't, then prayer would seem to have lost credibility. But religions usually have ways of explaining such failure. Maybe you or the sick person had done something horribly wrong, and this is God's punishment. Or maybe God just works in mysterious ways.

So far, then, we would expect the following kinds of memes to be survivors in the dog-eat-dog world of cultural evolution: claims that (a) are somewhat strange, surprising, counterintuitive,; (b) illuminate sources of fortune and misfortune; (c) give people a sense that they can influence these sources; (d) are by their nature hard to test decisively. In this light, the birth of religion doesn't seem so mysterious.

But doesn't our attraction to strangeness have its limits? It's one thing to believe that a man could survive a slide down a crumpling skyscraper through a series of lucky breaks. It's quite another to believe, with the Inuit (in chapter 1), that a sudden shortage of game is the work of a pouty female deity who lives at the bottom of the sea. In other words, "Man bites dog," however unlikely, seems more plausible than "God bites man."

But actually, the idea of a personal god or spirit who peevishly withholds food, or maliciously hurls lightning, get a boost from the evolved human brain. People reared in modern scientific societies may consider it only natural to ponder some feature of the world - the weather, say - and try to come up with a mechanistic explanation couched in the abstract language of natural law. But evolutionary psychology suggests that a much more natural way to explain anything is to attribute it to a humanlike agent. This is the way we're "designed" by natural selection to explain things. Our brain's capacity to think about causality - to ask why something happened and come up with theories that help us predict what will happen in the future - evolved in a specific context: other brains. When our distant ancestors first asked "Why," they weren't asking about the behavior of water or weather or illness; they were asking about the behavior of their peers.

That's a somewhat speculative (and, yes, hard-to-test!) claim. We have no way of observing our prehuman ancestors one or two or three million years ago, when the capacity to think explicitly about causality was evolving by natural selection. But there are ways to shed light on the process.

For starters, we can observe our nearest nonhuman relatives, chimpanzees. We didn't evolve from chimps, but chimps and humans do share a common ancestor in the not-too-distant past (4 to 7 million years ago). And chimps are probably a lot more like that common ancestor than humans. Chimps aren't examples of our ancestors circa 5 million BCE but they're close enough to be illuminating.

As the primatologist Frans de Waal has shown, chimpanzee society shows some clear parallels with human society. On of them is in the title of his book Chimpanzee Politics. Groups of chimps from coalitions - alliances - and the most powerful alliance gets preferred access to resources (notably a resource that in Darwinian terms is important: sex partners). Natural selection has equipped chimps with emotional and cognitive tools for playing this political game. One such tool is anticipation of a given chimp's future behavior based on past behavior. De Waal writes of a reigning alpha male, Yeroen, who face growing hostility from a growing ally named Luit: "He already sensed that Luit's attitude was changing and he knew that his position was threatened."

One could argue about whether Yeroen was actually pondering the situation in as clear and conscious a way as de Waal suggests. But even if chimps aren't quite up to explicit inference, they do seem close. If you imagine their politics getting more complex (more like, say, human politics), and them getting smarter (more like humans), you're imagining an organism evolving toward conscious thought about causality. And the causal agents about which these organisms will think are other such organisms, because the arena of causality is the social arena. In this realm, when a bad thing happens (like a challenge for Yeroen's alpha spot) or a good thing happens (like an ally coming to Yeroen's aid), it is another organism that is making the bad or good thing happen.

To be sure, other kinds of bad and good things happen to chimps: droughts, banana bonanzas, and so on. But there's no reason to think chimps are anywhere near consciously puzzling over those things - trying to anticipate droughts the way they try to anticipate the behavior of their neighbors. And there's no reason to think that our prehuman ancestors were, either. The best guess is that when natural selection built the mental machinery for predicatively pondering causality, the causal agents in question were peers - fellow prehumans. (Is he going to punch me? Is she going to betray me?) Moreover, when our ancestors first started talking about causality, they were probably talking about peers. (Why did you punch me? Do you know why she betrayed me?)

I'm not just talking about a habit. I'm not saying our ancestors were used to pondering questions of "Why?" by thinking about human beings. I'm suggesting that the human mind is built to do that - was "designed" by natural selection to do it.

So it's no surprise that when people first started expanding their curiosity, started talking about why bad and good things emanate from beyond the social universe, they came up with the kinds of answers that had made sense within their social universe. To answer a "why" question - such as "Why did the thunderstorm come just as that baby was being born?" - with anything other than a humanlike creature would have been kind of strange.

More than one hundred years ago Edward Tylor wrote that "spirits are simply personified causes," but he probably didn't appreciate, back then, how deeply natural personification is. Indeed, to talk about "personifying" causes is in a sense to get the story backward. Better to say that the modern scientific notion of a "cause" is a personified human being - or a depersonified god.

Even in modern science, the depersonification process may not be complete. Some philosophers believe that to chop the world up into "causes" and "effects" is to impose a falsely binary scheme on what is in fact a seamless reality. It may be that our "modern" way of thinking about causality still carries the vestiges of our primitive brains, still falsely reflects a social arena of causality, in which "causes" are distinct agents.

...

According to the book of Genesis, "God created man in his own image." According to Aristotle, "men created the gods after their own image." As should be clear by now, Aristotle seems to have been on to something, especially when it comes to the minds of gods. So, in theory, some of the more basic features of the human mind should be fairly standard equipment in gods, especially the gods of "primitive" religions.



That seems to be the case, and one of these features deserves special consideration: the part of the human mind shaped by the evolutionary dynamic known as "reciprocal altruism." In light of this dynamic, much about the origin of religion, and for that matter much about contemporary religion, makes a new kind of sense.


Thanks to reciprocal altruism, people are "designed" to settle into mutually beneficial relationships with other people, people whom they can count on for things ranging from food to valuable gossip to social support, and who in turn can count on them. We enter these alliances almost without thinking about it, because our genetically based emotions draw us in. We feel gratitude for a favor received, along with a sense of obligation, which may lead us to return the favor. We feel growing trust of and affection for people who prove reliable reciprocators (aka "friends"), which keep us entwined in beneficial relationships. This is what feelings like gratitude and trust are for - the reason they're part of human nature.

But of course, not everyone merits our trust. Some people accept our gifts of food and never reciprocate, or try to steal our mates, or exhibit disrespect in some other fashion. And if we let people thus take advantage of us day after day, the loses add up. In the environment of our evolution, these loses could have made the difference between surviving and not surviving, between prolifically procreating and barely procreating. So natural selection gave us emotions that lead us to punish the untrustworthy - people who violate our expectations of exchange, people who seem to lack the respect that a mutually beneficial relationship demands. They fill us with outrage, with moral indignation, and that outrage - working as "designed" - impels us to punish them in one way or another, whether by actually harming them or just by withholding future altruism. That will teach them! (Perhaps more important, it will also teach anyone else who is watching, and in the ancestral hunter-gatherer environment, pretty much everyone in your social universe was watching.)

This is the social context in which the human mind evolved: a world full of neighbors who, to varying degrees, are watching you for signs of betrayal or disrespect or dishonesty -  and who, should they see strong evidence of such things, will punish you. In such a social universe, when misfortune comes your way, when someone hits you or ridicules you or suddenly gives you the cold shoulder, there's a good chance it's because they feel you've violated the rules of exchange. Maybe you've failed to do them some favor they think they were due, or maybe you've shown them disrespect by doing something that annoys them.

Surely it is no coincidence that this generic explanation of why misfortune might emanate from a human being is also the generic explanation of why misfortune emanates from gods. In hunter-gatherer religions - and lots of other religions - when bad things happen, the root cause is almost always that people in one sense or another fail to respect the gods. They either fail to give gods their due (fail, say, to make adequate sacrifices to ancestral spirits), or they do things that annoy gods (like, say, making a noise while cicadas are singing). And the way to make amends to the aggrieved gods is exactly the way you'd make amends to aggrieved people: either give them something (hence ritual sacrifice), or correct future behavior so that it doesn't annoy them (quit making noises while cicadas are singing).

In this light, bizarre superstitions seem less bizarre. The Ainu, the indigenous hunter-gatherers of Japan, scrupulously refrained from spitting into fires. Strange! But if you accept their premise that hearth fires come courtesy of the fire goddess, the rest follows. You don't do things that insult people who give you gifts, because if you do they'll get in a snit and stop giving. And one thing that might well be taken as an insult is to spit on the gift.

Pascal Boyer believes that much of religion can be explained this way - a result of our attributing to supernatural causal agents the very human emotions that evolved to regulate reciprocal altruism: like our fellow human beings, gods are bent on enforcing their deals with us. This doesn't mean that the grievances of gods are always just. Evil deities, Boyer says, are "enforcers of unfair deals." But it's only natural that there should be such unfair gods; there are, after all, unfair people. (And people who can get away with being unfair - that is, can get more than they give - tend to be powerful, like gods.)

Two and a half millennia ago the Greek poet Xenophanes speculated that if horses had gods, these gods would be horses. Could be, but we'll never know, and in any event that's not quite the point being made here. It isn't that any imaginable intelligent species, in trying to explain mysterious things, would attribute them to beings like itself. It's that the history of the human species - notably including the evolution of the human brain in a context of reciprocal altruism, of social exchange - pointed in that direction. A law of the social jungle in which the human brain evolved is this: when bad things happen to you, it often means someone is mad at you, maybe because you've done something to offend them; making amends is often a good way to make the bad things stop happening. If you substitute "some god or spirit" for "someone," you have a law that is found in every known hunter-gatherer religion.

That religious ideas naturally appeal to the human mind doesn't, by itself, explain how religion got off the ground. Granted that religious "memes" have a "selective advantage" in cultural evolution, how exactly would a given meme - a particular religious belief - first take shape and gain momentum? We'll never know for sure, but human nature makes it easy to sketch a plausible scenario.

First, people like to command attention, and one way to do that is to place yourself at the center of dramatic events. In Mark Twain's The Adventures of Tom Sawyer, Tom Sawyer runs away with his friends Huckleberry Finn and Joe to play pirates on the Mississippi River, and the townspeople conclude that the boys have drowned. Twain describes their friends gathering and

talking in reverent tones of how Tom did so-and-so, the last time they saw him, and how Joe said this and that small trifle (pregnant with awful prophecy, as they could easily see now!) - and each speaker pointed out the exact spot where the lost lads stood at the time, and then added something like "and I was astanding just so - just as I am now, and as if you was him - I was as close as that - and he smiled, just this way - and then something seemed to go all over me, like - awful, you know - and I never thought what it meant, of course, but I can see now!"
Then there was dispute about who saw the dead boys last in life, and many claimed that dismal distinction, and offered evidences, more or less tampered with by the witness; and when it was ultimately decided who did see the departed last, and exchanged the last words with them, the lucky parties took upon themselves a sort of sacred importance, and were gaped at and envied by all the rest.

There is no reason to think that this incentive to claim special witness to high drama was any less powerful among hunter-gatherers circa 30,000 BCE than among midwestern Americans circa 1900 CE. Imagine that you are one of those hunter-gatherers and you walk past a place where someone died mysteriously, and you hear leaves rustle eerily. That's a story that will get people's attention, and you can heighten the attention by stressing how exquisitely timed the rustling was. And, by the way, didn't you catch sight of a shadowy - almost ethereal - creature out of the corner of your eye?

The anthropologist Stewart Guthrie has suggested that hunter-gatherers would be encouraged to make just such false sightings by standard human mental equipment - something called a "hyperactive agent-detection device." Because the costs of failing to detect a predator lurking in the woods are much higher than the costs of detecting one that isn't there, natural selection, he plausibly argues, may have biased our brains toward "false positives": you hear a rustling, your mind flashes the vivid hypothesis of some generic animal that's doing the rustling, and you turn toward it expectantly. Did you actually see something? Kind of.

In any event, if upon recounting your eerie encounter you get caught up in the spirit of the story and say you saw an ethereal being, then you may convince not just your audience, but yourself. One notable finding of modern psychology is how systematically misleading memory is. People often remember events wrongly from the get-go, and even when they don't, their memory can later be steered toward falsehood. In particular, the act of reporting false details can cement them firmly in mind. You don't just recount what you remember; you remember what you recount...This built-in fallibility makes sense from a Darwinian standpoint, allowing people to bend the truth self-servingly with an air of great and growing conviction. And, clearly, bent truths of a religious sort could be self-serving. If you were a close friend or relative of the deceased, then the idea that his powerful spirit is afoot may incline people to treat you nicely, lest they invite his wrath.

Another gem from social psychology: publicly espousing something not only helps convince you of its truth; it shapes your future perception, inclining you to see evidence supporting it but not evidence against it. So if you speculate that the strange, shadowy creature was the disgruntled spirit of the deceased, you'll likely find corroboration. You may notice that one of his enemies fell ill only a week after your sighting, while forgetting that one of his friends fell ill a few days earlier.

If you're a person of high status, all of this will carry particular weight, as such people are accorded unusual (and often undue) credibility. If, in a hunter-gatherer band of thirty people, someone widely esteemed claims to have seen something strange - and has a theory about what it was - twenty people may be convinced right off the bat. Then the aforementioned tendency of people to conform to peer opinion could quickly yield unanimity.

The number of mental tendencies involved in the creation and nourishment of religious falsehoods shouldn't surprise us. After all, The mind was built by a process that is, strictly speaking, indifferent to truth. Natural selection favors traits that are good at getting their bearer's genes into the next generation, period. If saying something false, or believing something false, often furthered that goal during human evolution, then the human mind will naturally encourage some kinds of falsity. This systematic muddle isn't an exclusive property of the "primitive" mind, as John Lubbock (chapter 1) suggested; all of the above delusory tendencies have been documented in people living in modern societies - many of them students at fine universities!

So why are people in modern societies so often aghast at "primitive" religion, so unable to comprehend how "primitive" belief got started? In part, it is the classic human failure of objectivity - an inability to see that your own beliefs may seem as strange to others as theirs seem to you. (An African Pygmy once responded to a missionary's description of heaven by asking, "How do you know?' Have you died and been there?") And in part it is a failure of imagination. Imagine that you are living in a small encampment surrounded by jungle or woodland or desert, entirely untouched by science and modern technology. Within the encampment, the social universe operates by largely intelligible laws; people don't generally, say, fly into a rage and assault their neighbors without a cause of some sort or another. But from outside this universe come mighty and momentous forces - storms, droughts, deadly animals, fatal illness. You are viscerally interested in explaining and controlling these things; you readily absorb and repeat any news or conjecture bearing on this goal. And, above all, you are only human. The rest is history.

...

In addition to our mental machinery for thinking consciously about causality - the machinery shaped by the evolution of reciprocal altruism - there are other innate tools for taking causality into account, and some of them operate almost entirely at the level of feeling.

For example, back when our ancestors didn't know that disease travels by microscopic organism, natural selection seems to have filled this knowledge gap, installing in our lineage an aversion to disease-carrying things. That is the conclusion psychologist Paul Rozin reached by studying disgust. It's no coincidence, he believes, that things which fill people everywhere with disgust - rotting corpses, excrement, putrid meat - are hazardous to our health.

However unsophisticated a feeling disgust may seem like, it actually entails a kind of metaphysics: a sense that some things are deeply impure and emit an invisible aura of badness, creating a dread zone. Pascal Boyer has suggested that disgust - our "contagion inference system" - may thus energize notions of ritual pollution that figure in many religions...

There is another feature of the human mind that may be involved in religious experience and that, like the "contagion inference system," is a way of taking account of causality without thinking consciously about it. In fact, it entered our lineage so long before consciously rational thought that it exists in all mammals. It is called "associative learning."

If a dog burns itself on rocks that surround a dying campfire, it will thereafter avoid such rocks. What is going on in the dog's mind is hard to say, but it probably isn't extended reflection on the causal link between fires and hot rocks, or between hot rocks and singed fur. Presumably the dog has just acquired something like a fear of those rocks, a fear that leads it to behave as if it understood the connection between rocks around dying campfires and singed fur. I once tried to walk a golden retriever past an intersection where, weeks earlier, she had been hit by a car. As we approached the intersection, she walked more and more slowly and warily until finally she came to a halt and started desperately resisting attempts to move her farther. It was as if, in her mind, the intersection was giving off a kind of spooky aura, and the closer she got to it, the stronger the aura felt.

Vestiges of this kind of crude learning mechanism in the human brain may incline people to see objects or places as inhabited by evil, a perception that figures in various religions. Hence, perhaps, the sense of dread that has been associated by some anthropologists with primitive religious experience.

And what of the sense of awe that has also been identified with religious experience - most famously by the German theologian Rudolf Otto (who saw primordial religious awe as often intermingled with dread)? Was awe originally "designed" by natural selection for some nonreligious purpose? Certainly feelings of that general type sometimes overtake people confronted by other people who are overwhelmingly powerful. They crouch abjectly, beg desperately for mercy. (In the Persian Gulf War of 1991, after weeks of American bombing, Iraqi soldiers were so shaken that they knelt and kissed the hands of the first Americans they saw even when those Americans were journalists.) On the one hand, this is a pragmatic move - the smartest thing to do under the circumstances. But it seems fueled at least as much by instinctive emotion as by conscious strategy. Indeed, chimpanzees do roughly the same thing. Faced with a formidable foe, they either confront it with a "threat display" or, if it's too formidable, crouch in submission.  

There's no telling what chimps feel in these instances, but in the case of humans there have been reports of something like awe. That this feeling is naturally directed toward other living beings would seem to lubricate theological interpretations of nature; if a severe thunderstorm summons the same emotion as an ill-tempered and potent foe, it's not much of a stretch to imagine an ill-tempered foe behind the thunderstorm.

Even chimpanzees may at times make a dim version of this conceptual leap. The primatologist Jane Goodall has observed chimps reacting to a rainstorm or a waterfall by making a threat display. She speculates that the "awe and wonder" that "underlie most religions" may be grounded in such "primeval, uncomprehending surges of emotion."

None of this is meant to deny the possibility of valid religious experience. The prospect that some states of consciousness move us closer to what mystics call "ultimate reality" - or even toward something worthy of the name "divine" - is hardly excluded by a scientific worldview. But defenders of religion would be ill advised to stake its validity on the claim, as Otto suggested in The Idea of the Holy, that at the dawn of religious history lies some mystical or revelatory experience that defies naturalistic explanation. Because the more we learn about the labyrinthine and sometimes irrational character of human nature, the easier it is to explain the origin of religion without invoking such a thing. Religion arose out of a hodgepodge of genetically based mental mechanisms designed by natural selection for thoroughly mundane purposes.

...the elements of early religion, though themselves of mundane origin, could through subsequent cultural evolution come to acquire a deeply, validly spiritual character. The idea isn't implausible. But how far humanity has traveled along the path of spiritual evolution is another question altogether.

The Evolution Of God. Wright, 460-483



26 Aug
No. A byproduct.

YES, AARON, RELIGION IS A BYPRODUCT OF BENEFICIAL ADAPTATIONS AND YOUR WIFE IS A NARCISSIST https://www.facebook.com/photo.php?fbid=608104559211884&l=a44de79516 JUST LIKE THAT BLACK ASSISTANT PROFESSOR AT SYRACUSE WHO I MENTION ON MY "I'LL EMBARRASS YOU" POST ON MY FOREVER FAME BLOG (This One Aaron https://twitter.com/charisseiscool).
http://scienceblogs.com/gregladen/2013/07/17/evolutionary-psychology-at-convergence-2013/
lOOK At My Nigga agoetz Goin' In On The Blogs. This Is A Man Who Takes His Evolutionary Psychology Seriously, Folkz.


Byproduct Theory

Many evolutionists hypothesize that religious thought is a byproduct of the normal function of nonreligious psychological mechanisms that evolved in ancestral contexts. Supporters of byproduct theory propose that these nonreligious adaptive mechanisms are responsible for religion’s incidental existence and generate the illusion of its ostensible design. This theory helps explain religion’s universality and cross-cultural similarity by outlining its cognitive foundation in adaptations such as theory of mind and other features of social intelligence. It also provides a viable explanation for the evolutionary origins of religious thought.

According to byproduct theorists, one cognitive adaptation that plays a key role in generating religious cognition is theory of mind, the human propensity to attribute mental states such as beliefs and intentions to others. Theory of mind allows humans to understand that the mental states of others can be different from their own. This provides the foundation for skills critical to social functioning, specifically in identifying cooperators, defectors, and cheaters in human social groups. Some scientists believe that the capacities that comprise social intelligence, including perspective taking and the ability to manipulate others, probably set off an evolutionary arms race between cooperative, defective, and cheating dispositions (Orbell, Morikawa, Hartwig, Hanley, & Allen, 2004). This naturally led to the development of the cooperative behaviors, ethical instincts, and moral systems that humans exhibit today.

Theory of mind and social intelligence also gave humans a new perspective on self and other. In particular, other is redefined to potentially include anything that can be assigned agency (e.g. family members, friends, enemies, the deceased, supernatural entities, etc.). One cognitive device critical to this process is a hyperactive agency detection mechanism. This mechanism interprets unusual or ambiguous stimuli from our surrounding environment as signs of agency. This often makes us perceive human-like characteristics in nonliving objects. This originally nonreligious mechanism would have been a valuable asset in avoiding predators, especially other hostile humans. Anthropologists such as Stewart Guthrie (1995) point to this systematic anthropomorphization of our environment (i.e. mistaking a shadow for an intentional spirit, seeing faces in the clouds, etc.) as the origins of supernaturalism and religious ideation. When coupled with a theory of mind, which readily assigns mental states and intentionality to these anthropomorphized objects, the resulting system incidentally generates thoughts of religious spirits and deities. Once humans developed these mechanisms, interactions among the mechanisms were inevitable, and the belief in the existence of immaterial, unverifiable, and supernatural entities with their own minds and intentions was a short cognitive step away.
http://www.evostudies.org/pdf/SmithVol2Iss2.pdf
http://whywereason.com/2011/10/18/why-people-believe-in-god-the-by-product-hypothesis/

We're Young, But Getting Old Before Our Time - Joe Stepping Out

"the presence of grandmothers was an evolutionary innovation that was important during human evolution...Grandmothers shouldered some of the energetic and care burden of their daughters' children...enabling an increase in the fertility of their daughters" a.co/d/2IzrDOh

She Did Her Job As A Grandmother And It Paid Off! She Had G00D Reproductive Success (The Only Success That Matters) Even Though She Lost A Grandson!

"I HEARD ALL THEM KIDZ BACK LIVIN' AT GRANNIES!" - JOE

my great grandmother who'll be 100 next month (on the right) & her best friend who is 102! 71 years of friendship ✨💫
https://twitter.com/Kamaricopeland/status/696925447213420544
Have You Noticed That A Higher Than Average Percentage Of The Black Female Population Has Genes That Predispose Them To Longevity (Living Longer Than The Average Female)? Have You Also Noticed That These Long Lived Back Females (Grandmothers, Great Grandmothers) Tend To Have Greater Reproductive Success (They Themselves Have More Offspring And Their Offspring Have More Offspring)? Read Below!

While every woman's body and brain react differently in the years after menopause, for many this is a time of increasing freedom and control over our lives. Impulses are less likely to confuse or agitate us. Our survival may no longer depend on a steady paycheck, and there's less value in pretending about how we feel and more in presenting and living our passionate, real selves. Helping others and engaging in solving serious problems in the world can energize us. This is also a time when grandmothering can bring new, often uncomplicated joy. Maybe life does save some of the best for last. My sixty-year-old patient Denise, for example, had always been an independent woman focused on her marketing career, even while she was raising her two children. When her daughter gave birth for the first time, Denise ws unprepared, she told me, for the waves of love she felt for her grandchild. "I was completely swept off my feet," she said, "which I never, ever expected. I've got a million things going on in my life, but for some reason I can't get enough of this baby. And my daughter's letting me into her life in a way that she has never before. She needs me now, and I want to be there for her."

The special, supportive role that grandmothers play may be one of the reasons that evolution engineered women to live for decades after they can no longer bear children. Grandmothers, according to the University of Utah anthropologist Kristen Hawkes, may actually be one of the keys to growth and survival in ancient human populations. Hawkes argues that in the Stone Age, the extra food-gathering efforts of able-bodied postmenopausal women increased the survival rate of young grandchildren. Grandmothers' provisioning and help also enabled younger women to produce more children at shorter intervals, increasing the populations fertility and reproductive success. Even though the lifespan in hunter-gatherer societies is typically less than forty, about a third of all adult women survive past that age, and may go on to live productively into their sixties and seventies. Among the Hadza hunter-gatherer population in Tanzania, Hawkes found, for example, that hardworking grandmothers in their sixties spent more time foraging than did younger mothers, providing foor for their grandchildren and increasing their chances of survival. Researchers have found similar positive effects of grandmothers among Hungarian gypsies and populations in India and Africa. In rural Gambia, in fact, anthropologists found that the presence of a grandmother improves a child's prospects for survival much more than the presence of a father. In other words, women at menopaus, the world over, have the option to embrace the life-sustaining role of grandmother, too. (The Female Brain)  

 https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=lIJ3xCR-PhU
 CARTA: Childrearing in Human Evolution -- Kristen Hawkes: Grandmothers and the Extended Family

"RAAAZED BY HER GRANDMOTHER" - Lil C.S. Lewis

Feb 17
Today I lost a woman I will always love RIP MINE #1 grandmother hard ass pill to swallow
SWALLOOOOOOW! GRANDMOTHERS PLAY AN ESPECIALLY VITAL ROLE IN THE FUNCTIONING AND MAINTENANCE OF BLACK AMERICAN FAMILIES. THEY ASSUME THE RESPONSIBILITIES OF THE FATHER AND SOMETIMES THE MOTHER*, SINCE BOTH ARE OFTEN ABSENT IN THE LIVES OF THEIR CHILDREN. 

*THE BLACK MOTHER MAY LIVE AT HOME WITH THE KIDS (MAY PHYSICALLY BE PRESENT), BUT SOCIALLY AND EMOTIONALLY SHE MAY AS WELL BE A MILLION MILES AWAY. BLACK GRANDMOTHERS FILL THE SOCIAL AND EMOTIONAL VOID LEFT BY THESE UNFIT MOTHERS.


1h
Thank God I didn't breast feed. Hungry ass baby lol
PERFECT EXAMPLE OF A BLACK MOTHER. MOST OF THEM ARE UNFIT TO BE MOTHERS, YET THEY PRODUCE ALMOST AS MANY CHILDREN AS MEXICANS (WHAT A HORRIBLE MOTHER). SHE'S SETTING HER CHILD UP FOR LONG TERM FAILURE AND A DISAPPOINTING LIFE (INFANTS THAT AREN'T BREASTFED SUFFER A NUMBER OF BEHAVIORAL, PSYCHOLOGICAL, PHYSIOLOGICAL, AND PHYSICAL* DISORDERS THAT HAMPER THEM IN BOTH THEIR EARLY YEARS AND LATER LIFE). WHY BRING A CHILD INTO THE WORLD IF YOU'RE GOING TO FUCK UP THEIR LIFE FROM THE GET GO? SHE'S JUST ANOTHER DUMB NIGGERETTE. (LEMME SMOKE ME ONE UH THEM NIGGERETTES MY NIGGA!)

*LONG FACE SYNDROME IS ONE SUCH DISORDER. LET GO

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=d25lcmlQT_Y
 CARTA: Culture-Gene Interactions in Human Origins: Kristen Hawkes - The Grandmother Hypothesis

Last among sexual mysteries in this chapter, as in life, we come to menopause. Less engaging than orgasm, less obvious than breasts, menopause shares more with menstruation than its first three letters (which don't refer to male human beings, but to "month"). Like menstruation, menopause is semisecret and hormonally underwritten. It is the matching bookend to a woman's reproductive life: from menarche to menopause. And like menstruation, menopause can also be downright troublesome, substituting hot flashes for monthly cramps.

Biologists, too, are discomforted by menopause, since it presents us with yet another evolutionary conundrum. We've already noted that reproduction is the sine qua non of evolutionary success, which makes it especially perplexing that women's reproductive spigots are turned off at what seems an inappropriately early age. Most animals do not experience a prolonged life stage during which they are alive yet nonbreeding. So long as they draw breath, they typically release eggs. But women stop ovulating within just a few years of age 50, when they may still have a few decades of vigorous and for the most part healthy life ahead of them.

Men keep producing sperm into their eighth and even ninth decades. For women, it isn't even a question of making eggs, since every girl is born with all the eggs she will ever have, roughly 400; they simply have to mature and then be released. The "how" of menopause is well understood. A woman's reproductive spigot is literally turned off by a dramatic reduction in endocrine hormones, notably estrogen. But this is proximate causation. What about the "why"? Why has selection favored women whose endocrine machinery runs down when it does? What are the ultimate, evolutionary reasons?

There is no reason to suppose that age and eggs are necessarily incompatible: Female African elephants breed into their 60s and blue whales into their 90s. Not only that, but in Homo sapiens, eggs - like sperm - aren't produced de novo throughout life. Maybe that's the answer: At some point, each woman just runs out of eggs.

≠Chateau Emissary≠ ‏@ChateauEmissary 5 hours ago
Why did menopause evolve? Executive summary: younger mothers are better at mothering than older mothers.

This "explanation" turns out to be no explanation at all, however, since once again it confuses proximate with ultimate causation. If there were a reproductive payoff to reproducing in one's 50s, 60s, or 70s, you can rest assured that girls would be born with 500, 600, or 700 eggs, instead of their current 400 or so. Not only that, but women who use birth control pills - which inhibit ovulation - and who therefore only release one half to one third of the lifetime egg supply nonetheless enter menopause just like everyone else, despite having all those unused eggs, and no later than their sisters who supposedly became menopausal because they'd used up all of theirs.

More important, eggs eventually go "bad," causing the risk of genetic defects to increase with maternal age. According to the March of Dimes, for example, a 25-year-old woman has about a 1 in 1,250 chance of having a baby with Down syndrome; a 30-year-old has a 1 in 1,000 chance; a 35-year-old, 1 in 400; a 40-year-old, 1 in 100; and a 45-year-old woman is more than 40 times more likely to produce a baby with Down syndrome. Looked at in terms of actual risk, however, the data are much less overwhelming: Even a 45-year-old has a 29 in 30 chance of giving birth to a child who does not have Down syndrome! Whereas there is a genuine genetic risk to reproducing in one's fifth or sixth decade, sheer mathematics nonetheless suggests that the potential genetic payoff greatly exceeds the possible downside. 

...

...Why should it matter to evolution if women kept reproducing, or trying to do so, until they died in the process? After all, this is precisely what happens among nearly all other species, which typically breed until the bitter end. It must somehow be the case that women who, having reached a certain age, desisted from reproduction actually ended up leaving more genetic descendants than those who kept on keeping on. But how?

What follows are some ideas about why natural selection might have favored early termination of women's fertility. First comes simple prudence, as negatively modeled by Jane Goodall's famous chimpanzee matriarch, Flo. Chimps do not undergo menopause, and Flo...kept breeding into advanced old age. It must have seemed a near miracle when Flo became pregnant for what turned out to be the last time, since she was obviously dilapidated in every respect. Here is the sad story, reported by the Jane Goodall institute:
Flo gave birth to at least five offspring: Faban, Figan, Fifi, Flint, and Flame. She was a wonderful, supportive, affectionate and playful mother to the first three. But she looked very old when the time came to wean young Flint, and she had not fully succeeded  in weaning him when she gave birth to Flame. By this time she seemed exhausted and unable to cope with the aggressive demands and tantrums of Flint, who wanted to ride on her back and sleep with her even after the birth of his new sister. She still had not weaned Flint when Flame died at the age of six months, and at this point stopped even trying to push Flint to independence. Flint therefore became abnormally dependent on his old mother. When Flo died in 1972, he was unable to cope without her. He stopped eating and interacting with others and showed signs of clinical depression. Soon thereafter, Flint's immune system became too weak to keep him alive. He died at the age of eight and a half, within one month of losing his mother Flo.


Flo wasn't really a failure, since she produced at least three flourishing offspring. But the likelihood is that if she had been just a wee bit more reproductively prudent - if she had refrained from that last breeding attempt - she wouldn't merely have survived longer (which, after all, isn't an evolutionary payoff in itself), but so, too, would the unfortunate Flame and perhaps Flint as well. Maybe, therefore, we shouldn't speak of Flo's failure, but rather, her folly. And maybe human menopause is a way that evolution has outfitted our own matriarchs with a way to avoid Flo's folly, by forcing them to be reproductively prudent and getting their bodies to "just say no."

Even in medically sophisticated societies, a 40-year-old woman faces seven times more risk of dying in childbirth than does a 20-year-old. Hard-nosed evolutionary biologists might be nonetheless unimpressed, however, pointing out that as with the payoff of reproducing despite the increased risk of genetic anomalies, selection would still favor a woman who tried, even if she failed, simply because it would favor any who succeeded. But this omits another important consideration, somewhat valid for chimpanzees but more so for human beings: the extent to which offspring survival (and thus parental fitness) depends on parental investment in those offspring. Since children depend so heavily on their parents, human parents may well have been under especially strong selection pressure to be prudent rather than go with the Flo.

In the poker game of breeding - in which maximizing your fitness substitutes for maximizing your pile of chips - just as there is a payoff for betting successfully on one's breeding potential, there is a cost to betting too high, like Flo did. Similarly, it would be suboptimal to be too prudent and bet too low - that is, to quit breeding too soon - thereby underplaying one's hand. As in Kenny Roger's song The Gambler, "you got to know when to hold 'em, know when to fold 'em, know when to walk away and know when to run." The prudent mother hypothesis is that menopause tells women when to fold 'em. But it's not the only game in town. Another is called the grandmother hypothesis.

The basic idea is simple enough, although hidden within is a crucially important revision in our current understanding of evolution and, indeed, of the very nature of living things. First, consider that a woman begins to experience menopause at about the time when her own children, born perhaps two decades or so earlier, are themselves likely to become parents. That is, she may well be - or is about to become - a grandmother. The grandmother hypothesis, then , is that by foregoing reproduction, especially at a time when the cost of reproductive "imprudence" is rising - higher risk of morbidity and mortality during pregnancy and childbirth along with increased prospects of genetic anomalies in any offspring actually produced - a middle-aged women might be freeing herself to contribute to the eventual success of her grandchildren. By doing so, she is actually being genetically selfish as much as altruistic, since the beneficiaries of her personal reproductive restraint include not only the grandchildren themselves but also their genes - which is to say, the grandmother's too.

The surprising fact that people, in general, feel better at 70 years than at 17 may be an evolutionary adaptation to facilitate grandparenting.

It could be mere coincidence or - more likely - part of evolution's design that around the world, grandparents in general and grandmothers in particular pitch in and help out. Not only that, but those who do so typically end up with more grandchildren than those who don't. The grandmother hypothesis does not preclude the hypothesis of prudent mothering, however, since once a mother is no longer encumbered with dependent children, it makes social as well as biological sense that she would be inclined to help out with her kids' kids.

Once again, our species' unusually long period of profound juvenile dependency may also be involved, insofar as such neediness would confer a special benefit to assistance rendered by others beyond the parental pair. Consistent with this, Sarah Hrdy has proposed that humanity may well have evolved in the context of extensive cooperative parenting. And who would be more qualified and also better positioned to gain biologically as a result, than grandmothers? 

Detailed studies by anthropologist Kristen Hawkes and her colleagues have found that among the Hadza, modern hunter-gatherers of Tanzania, the men hunt while the women gather and forage and (ta da!) the most energetic and productive foragers of all are postmenopausal women. A young mother, no matter how healthy and hard-working, is necessarily constrained while burdened with a baby, making such assistance nothing to sneeze at. Sure enough, Hadza grandmothers give their bounty to their children and grandchildren, whose body weights vary directly with their grandmothers' food-gathering efforts. It is doubtless significant that among the Hadza studied by Hawkes and her colleagues, every nursing mother had a postmenopausal helper.

The technical term for cooperative breeding is "allo-parenting" (allo = "other") and its likely importance in our evolution should give pause when we consider the extent to which modern Western societies - with their assumption of the "nuclear family" - make it impossible for grandparents to make the kind of social and biological contribution that might well have been crucial for 99% of our biological past. Although multigenerational households can certainly introduce their own forms of stress, it can hardly be denied that children, parents, and grandparents (perhaps grandmothers in particular) have also gained greatly from the interaction.

How much, we cannot tell. But the basic pattern, in which hard-working grandmothers contribute significantly to the success of their grandchildren, has been confirmed by other anthropologists studying other human groups. All of this makes it increasingly likely that grandmothers own their nonreproductive status to the payoffs that - at least in the past - they were able to convey, and the genetic recompense they received as a result.

An interesting twist to the grandmother hypothesis also merits our attention. It is deservedly popular to point to "win-win solutions" by which everyone in a competition - better yet, an interaction - comes out ahead. The sad reality, however, is that life is often a zero-sum game in which benefit to one participant necessitates some cost to another. The simple act of reproducing, and more important to the point successfully rearing one's offspring, is often zero sum, especially when resources are scarce. So perhaps we should consider the role of menopause as a way of minimizing reproductive competition, something particularly relevant when one individual's baby making can depress that of another.

The "competition avoidance" hypothesis argues that menopause is how middle-aged women avoid competing with younger women - by opting out altogether. Once again, they need not be doing so out of genuine altruism since those younger women who benefit are typically either the menopausal woman's daughters or daughter-in-law, so that in either case, the woman whose ovaries say "no" may well be saying "yes" to her genes, each of which is likely, with a probability of .25, to be present in her grandchildren.

According to the grandmother hypothesis, among the payoffs received by grandmothers themselves are benefits that go beyond emotional gratification and satisfied love and that include evolutionary payoffs received by the menopausal woman's genes. Taking a "gene's eye" view of evolution, natural selection does not proceed with the individual in mind, but rather, the gene. As biologist Richard Dawkins has pointed out so cogently, bodies aren't the bottom line in evolution; genes are. Bodies don't last beyond a single generation; genes do. And so, when at a certain age women forego reproducing (i.e. when they commence menopause) and also begin being helpful grandmothers, their genes are "selfishly" looking out for copies of themselves, genes "for" menopause that get projected into the future via those additional grandchildren that are benefited. 

Sarah Hrdy tells of a particular langur monkey, "old Sol," who had ceased cycling and thus might have been quasimenopausal, in a sense. She was obviously decrepit and marginalized within her group, living a sad, solitary, and - it appeared - increasingly useless end of life until a strange adult male invaded the langur troop and attempted to work infanticidal mayhem. Writes Hrdy,
It was Sol who repeatedly charged this sharp-toother male nearly twice her weight to place herself between him and the threatened baby. When the infanticidal male seized the infant in his jaws and ran off with him, Sol pursued the attacker and wrested the wounded baby back. With danger momentarily past, and the wounded infant once again in his mother's arms, old Sol resumed her diffident attitude. That an arthritic old female would become marginalized with age is scarcely surprising. More curious was Sol's transformation from decrepit outcast to intrepid defender.
It doesn't diminish Sol's courage to point out that by defending youngsters, some of whom may be her own grandchildren, Sol and other warrior grandmothers may literally be justifying their own postreproductive existence, or - to put it differently - their genes are acting out their own payoff.

Even then, the grandmother hypothesis is not literally proven. It seems likely, however, that menopause may serve to keep middle-aged women from reproducing at a time when their personal risk is increasing (higher mortality) and payoff is decreasing (greater danger of producing genetically defective offspring), so that these women are more fit in the evolutionary sense if they care for those children already produced (prudent mothering) as well as contributing to their own successful grandmotherhood. Even women who have no children would presumably be influenced by the same basic evolutionary pressures, since for most of our species' history, intentional childlessness was not an option. In addition, once our conceptual focus has shifted to what is presumably natural selections' focus as well - the gene rather than the individual - it isn't strictly necessary for someone to have children to receive an evolutionary benefit.


Homo Mysterious: Evolutionary Puzzles of Human Nature. Barash, p. 64-72.


The Grandmother effect in numbers: "Having a living maternal grandmother increased the number of offspring born by their daughters by about 20 per cent. It also had a positive impact on the number of grandchildren that survived to age 15."

"the presence of grandmothers was an evolutionary innovation that was important during human evolution...Grandmothers shouldered some of the energetic and care burden of their daughters' children...enabling an increase in the fertility of their daughters" a.co/d/2IzrDOh


2h
My grandma is having some health issues please say a prayer for her! Prayer is powerful and she needs it.
OH, ABUELA, MIJO. PRAY FOR ABUELA, MIJO. BOW YOUR HEAD, CLASP YOUR HANDS AND PRAY HARD ESE. THAN FIND YOU A CURANDERO AND HAVE HIM PUT HIS HANDS ON HER, A MAGICAL POTION ON HER, AND A SPELL ON HER.



 https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=qZvxu0zVhdA
 Dr. Kristen Hawkes - "Grandmothers and Human Evolution" (2013 Margo Wilson Memorial Lecture)

http://www.evolutionnews.org/2013/01/more_evolutiona068771.html




Today is my grandmother's 86th Birthday. Happy Birthday Grandma. #86#september 

Have You Noticed That A Higher Than Average Percentage Of The Black Female Population Has Genes That Predispose Them To Longevity (Living Longer Than The Average Female)? Have You Also Noticed That These Long Lived Back Females (Grandmothers, Great Grandmothers) Tend To Have Greater Reproductive Success (They Themselves Have More Offspring And Their Offspring Have More Offspring)? Read Above!
"My Grand Ma Foe Hunned N Seven D Fi!" - Suga Booga #Bitch


and you know what i hate THE MOST!? women are the ones that write this shit!!!!!!!!! how fucked up it that!?!?

Because It Takes One To Know One. Most Women Understand Other Women Better Than Men Understand Women, So From This Perspective They Can Write More Accurately About Women (At Least If They're Honest With Themselves). In Other Words, Women Who Are Honest With Themselves And Not Afraid To Tell The Truth Even If It Turns People Against Them WILL WRITE THAT WOMEN ARE MORE EMOTIONAL AND LESS RATIONAL THAN MEN (ESPECIALLY WHEN MAKING DECISIONS) AND THEREFORE MORE TEMPERAMENTAL* (They Evolved To Be This Way, Even Though Most People Who Write About Women Are Unaware Of This). (Iggy, I've NEVER Heard Of You, So You're A NOBODY In My Book, But I Know One Thing About You. You Don't Know Anything About Human Evolution, Specifically Why Females Evolved The Personality Traits And Disposition They Evolved, Nor Do You Understand What Drives You Genetically And Culturally To Be A Power Bitch (I'll Give You A Hint, Though. You're Not Dependent Upon Males. You're Economically Self-Sufficient And Self-Sustaining, Which Ultimately Means You're Controlling Your Reproductive Destiny And That Empowers You). Here, Get A Dose Of This: http://www.evoyage.com/Evolutionary%20Feminism/CoreBeliefsEvoFeminism.html)

*Why Are Women More Temperamental? Because They're The Choosier Sex. They're The Sex That Incurs The Greater Reproductive Costs (9 Months Of Pregnancy And Child Rearing), So They Evolved To Be More Discriminating, More Discerning, More Fickle, More Finicky, And More Moody (All Traits That Make Them Better Mate Selectors). SELECTA!

No, I Said I Was A Liberal, Feminine, Feminist!

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=V1flXTxuwPI
 CARTA:Childrearing in Evolution--Sarah Blaffer Hrdy: Born Human: How the Utterly Dependent Survive

Working mothers are not new. For most of human existence, and for millions of years before that, primate mothers have combined productive lives with reproduction. This combination of work with motherhood has always entailed tradeoffs. Mothers either sustained energetic costs and lost efficiency by toting babies everywhere (the way baboons and !Kung mothers do) or else located an alloparent to take on the task. What is new for modern mothers, though, is the compartmentalization of their productive and reproductive lives. The factories, laboratories, and offices where women in post-industrial societies go to "forage" are even less compatible with childcare than jaguar-infested forests and distant groves of mongongo nuts reached by trekking across desert.

The economic reality of most people's lives today is that families require more than one wage-earner -- or forager. Single parents are especially hard-pressed to make ends meet. Only brief periods of prosperity or isolated blips of elite privilege have made this untrue for some people during a few periods in human existence. An expansion of the U.S. economy after World War II, for example, meant that many married women could afford to stay home with their babies. But no more. Most Mothers, even if they want to, do not have the option of staying home to care for their babies.

 LACTATIN'
(Breastsis For Breakfast!)



And that's the modern rub. During the Pleistocene, women could carry their babies as they foraged or gathered firewood. Dual-career mothers still strive to balance their subsistence needs against the time, energy, and resources needed to rear their children. But the physical (if not always the emotional) environment in which these compromises must be made is considerably different from the workplace of our ancestors. In some respects, omnipresent conflicts create even more tension today than in the past, because the incentives to fix them strike mothers as optional. Outcomes are measured in terms of the personal toll -- insecurities among infants, stress in their mothers -- rather than increased mortality. Simply put, the pressures to change are less intense when children can (literally) live with the consequences.

If infants feel stressed by the separation, so do millions of working mothers. At the same time, the evolutionarily novel modern workaday world opened the door for untrammeled expression of another ancient female motivation -- striving for status, or, in the case of a forager, what one might think of as "local clout."


It is widely assumed that competitiveness, status-striving, and ambition, qualities that are essential for success in demanding careers, are incompatible with being a "good mother," who is expected to be selfless and nurturing. "There is no getting around the fact that ambition is not a maternal trait. Motherhood and ambition are still largely seen as opposing forces," states Shari Thurer, a prominent contemporary psychologist. Sociologists can document at length the "cultural contradictions" produced by women combining motherhood with jobs in the American workplace.

Under conditions of the modern world, and if we assume the old definition of mothering as an innately charitable and selfless pursuit, the point is well taken. But as I described i chapter 2, mothering in the natural world is different from the Victorian image of mothers. Mothers' work has not always been so compartmentalized from child-rearing as it is today, nor her status so separate from the prospects that a mother's offspring would survive and prosper. 

Modern women may think of status as the icing on their economic cake. But once the significance of social rank is understood for such vital functions as a mother keeping another female from eating her baby (as in the case of chimps), or from keeping another female from monopolizing resources needed by her own offspring (as in the case of other cooperatively breeding mammals), the struggle for status seems more nearly a foothold on posterity than a frill. "Ambition" was an integral part of producing offspring who survived and prospered. 

Establishing an advantageous niche for herself was how Flo, the chimpanzee female that Jane Goodall studied for so many years, stayed fed, guaranteed access to food for her offspring, and kept them safe from interference by other mothers. Eventually, Flo's high status made it possible for her daughter Fifi to be among the few females who would remain in her natal place to breed -- in Fifi's case, inheriting her mother's territory. Even more impressive data documenting the connection between female status and all sorts of reproductive parameters -- age of menarche, infant-survival rates, and even sex ratios of offspring -- have been compiled for Old World cercopithecene monkeys like macaques and baboons. These data strongly suggest that generalized striving for local clout was genetically programmed into the psyches of female primates during a distant past when status and motherhood were totally convergent.   


Evidence for human primates is less clear, in part because husbands figure so prominently in the social status of most mothers. Yet both fiction and ethnography provide multiple examples. For example, Nisa, the !Kung woman, tells what happened when her first husband, Tashay, brought home a second wife. "I chased her away and she went back to her parents," Nisa says simply. Nisa's own mother had done the same thing a generation before. The children of this new wife would have competed with Nisa's for food provided by her husband and other community members. Nisa acted so as to maintain her status as her husband's only, primary wife. Her actions were in keeping with being a "good mother." Such women do not compete for status and reputation in the spheres that matter to men (for example, being known as a great hunter or warrior); they compete in spheres that actually matter to mothers.   


Occasionally, we can detect bizarre manifestations of these old connections, as in the case of the Texas mother who hired a hit-man to murder the mother of her daughter's cheer-leading rival in order to derail her emotionally. But for the most part, status-striving by mothers seeking to enhance the prospects of their children is more subtle. Think of the womanly rivalries chronicled for early-nineteenth-century England by Jane Austen, or by Edith Wharton for the early-twentieth-century tribal life of "Old New York." In subtle, private, and scarcely perceptible ways, both mothers and their relations close ranks so as to promote and protect the marriage opportunities (which in that world meant access to resources) of young kinswomen, while locking out other young women. We tend to think of these mothers as "controlling," "pushy," "interfering" -- and I don't disagree -- but the venerable ancestry of such traits is worth considering. In their environment of evolutionary relevance, these women would have been behaving like successful mothers.


Far from "opposing forces," maternity and ambition are inseparably linked. The circumstances of modern life tend, however, to obscure the connection. This is because jobs, status, and resource defense occur in separate domains from child-rearing. At the same time, civilized mores and laws mean that mothers do not have to rely on intimidation to drive off rival mothers and keep their offspring safe from competing interests. Most mothers reading this book worry far less about famine, tigers, and infanticidal conspecifics than they worry over job promotions, health benefits, and finding adequate daycare. 


For the most part, mothers striving for status in the modern workplace do so outside the home. Often working mothers are driven to pursue status interests for long hours, far from home, in ways just as likely to harm as to help their baby cope with life. The conflict, however, is not between maternity and ambition, but between the needs of infants and the way a woman's ambition plays out in the modern workplace.


In the modern world, status (whether socioeconomic or professional) is, if anything, inversely correlated with reproductive success. This is especially true for women who earn their status. Not long ago, sociobiologist Susan Essock-Vitale looked at the reproductive success of people listed on Forbes Magazine's annual listing of the four hundred wealthiest Americans. Those women who had inherited wealth had significantly more children on average than successful businesswomen who had acquired their wealth through their own efforts. This should not come as a surprise. When given the opportunity, many women value upward mobility over time devoted to rearing a family. We need only look at the grueling hours that working mothers put into jobs as lawyers, doctors, and research scientists, careers with demands as insatiable as those of children. But if our evolutionary heritage has any relevance to what we are, how can this be?

The answer is simple. In worlds where there was no birth control, and where no female was ever celibate, there was no possibility that female rank and maternal reproductive success could be other than correlated. Nature built in no safeguards to ambition run awry, as it were, to energies diverted to status ends that were not linked to the production, survival, and prosperity of offspring. Now that status and the survival of offspring have been decoupled, will there be selection against women who are especially inclined or driven to achieve? Probably, if our species survives long enough, and if circumstances in the work place don't change.

Torn between two ancient, pressing, and now incompatible urges, women are forced to make new tradeoffs. Forging workable compromises between infant needs and maternal ambition requires considerable ingenuity, self-understanding, and common sense. This is especially true in highly competitive and demanding fields. Science provides the case studies with which I am most familiar.

In 1976, the year I completed my Ph.D., an article appeared titled "The high price of success in science," written by a young molecular biologist, Nancy Hopkins, who would go on to become a leader in the field. She argued that it was not impossible for a woman in such a competitive profession -- demanding, in her case, a minimum of seventy hours a week in the lab -- to "be a successful wife and mother as well as a successful scientist." Her words were sobering and, looking back on that era of what Hopkins called "the bionic woman hard sell of the '70s," unusually honest. About the time Hopkins wrote that article, there were ten tenured women professors at Harvard Medical School; nine of the ten had no children.

Yet there were women who managed to successfully combine science with motherhood. None I know took ordinary routes. Mary Jane West-Eberhard, for example, whose ideas about the role of development in evolutionary processes I discussed in chapter 3, is legendary among women field biologists for the way she managed to combine her family and professional lives. "It's what we all do," she told me once, apropos of the extra-tough compromises field-biology requires of mothers. "We each construct our own idiosyncratic life." In West-Eberhard's case, she opted to forgo a conventional teaching position at a major university to take a research job. The position allowed the Eberhards to live in Central America, where they could afford housekeeping help, and, more importantly, where they could do research while keeping an eye on their three children -- literally, since the wasps they studied were on the roof of their house.

My own compromises took me in the opposite direction. I switched from tramping around forested hillsides, following monkeys in India, to doing research on human parents in archives in the United States, where I used part-time daycare, and along with my husband took full advantage of emerging opportunities to work less than full-time and to use fax machines, and eventually the internet, to work at home. When my third child was born, I hired a generous-hearted allomother on a very long-term basis. She lives with us still, though  my youngest child is twelve, and the allomother now pursues a part-time profession of her own.

Pretty obviously, not one of us was living the same way as our ancestors. Yet we were required to resolve similar dilemmas, and so we forged new solutions for doing so -- a theme that recurs throughout this book.

Many of us at different stages of our lives desperately desire a child. Others, out of commitment to career or for other reasons, are determined to have none. Many women are certain they will never want a child, and  then change their mind. Still others have babies by accident. Those who consciously decide are often making pragmatic decisions with a watchful eye on the effects upon their career, existing children, or the overall well-being of their family. Few people give much thought to the evolutionary origins of the emotions that inform such "decisions." But I am convinced we are more tightly linked to our past than most people imagine.

Whether we think about it or act "on impulse," each of us constantly makes myriad small decisions on a daily basis that in ancestral environments would have been correlated with reproductive success. Like it or not, each of us lives with the emotional legacy and decision-making equipment of mothers who acted so as to ensure that at least one offspring survived to reproduce. Prudent allocation of reproductive effort and the construction of an advantageous social niche in which her offspring could survive and prosper were linked to ultimate reproductive success.


 Mother Nature: A History of Mothers, Infants, and Natural Selection. Blaffer Hrdy, p. 109-114.


DUPLICITOUS

By the end of the twentieth century, the role of Flo, Jane Goodall's most endearing mother chimp, was expanded and recast. Flo's evident tenderness and patience were only part of the story about her success as a mother. If in this book I fail to stress sufficiently this nursing component, the reason is that I assume that it is already well known, widely described, and commonly assumed. But there are secrets to Flo's reproductive success that are less well known, less often quoted. These include Flo's ability to carve out for herself a secure and productive territory deep within the boundaries patrolled by the Gombe males. Many of these males were former sexual consorts; others were her own sons who had risen to a high rank in the fluctuating local hierarchy. Flo was as secure a female chimp could be from outside males who from time to time would raid her community, and if they could, kill not just unrelated infants but adult males and older females as well.

But Flo did more than commandeer a productive larder and keep her offspring safe. She supported her offspring politically, permitting Fifi to translate her mother's advantages into her own. At Flo's death, Fifi parlayed her mother's local connections into the inestimable privilege of philopatry, remaining in her natal place. Philopatry (which means literally "loving one's home country") meant that instead of migrating away to find a new place to live, Fifi -- like half of all females born at Gombe -- managed to stay where she was born. Fifi continued to use her mother's rich, familiar larder, and enjoy the protection of male kin.

Make no mistake, reproductively, nothing becomes a female more than remaining among kin. The advantage, Fifi began breeding at an unusually early age, and so  far has produced seven successive offspring, six surviving -- the all-time record for lifetime reproductive success in a wild Great Ape female. She also holds the record for shortest interval between surviving births ever reported in wild chimps. Her second-born son, Frodo, has grown into the largest male on record at Gombe and ranks in the status hierarchy just below the current alpha male, Fifi's firstborn son, Freud, while Fanni, Fifi's third-born, holds the record for the earliest ever anogenital swelling, at 8.5 years. Thus does Flo's family prosper.

Early on, Goodall and her students noticed that when Flo approached other females, they gave nervous pant-grunts and moved out of her way. Females could be divided into those that held sway and those that gave way. What Goodall did not immediately grasp, however, was why female rank was so important. We know that, given the opportunity, a more dominant female chimp will kill and eat babies born to other females.

Over the decades, that records were kept at Gombe, at least four, possibly as many as ten, newborn infants were killed by females. When Goodall reported the first two cases of infant killing and cannibalism by another mother in 1977, the so-called crimes of a female named Passion, she, like most people, assumed that the female killing these infants must be deranged. A few sociobiologists suspected otherwise and suggested that females from a more dominant lineage were "eliminating a competitor while the infant was still sufficiently vulnerable to be dispatched with impunity."

From the 1970s onward, isolated cases of infanticide by rival mothers continued to be reported for other species of social mammals -- ground squirrels, prairie dogs, wild dogs, marmosets, some fifty species in all. Most cases were attributed either to a mineral deficiency or protein lust by a hungry female (since in some cases victims were eaten) or to mothers clearing out a niche and thereby making resources available for her own breeding efforts -- a model first proposed by sociobiologist Paul Sherman at Cornell University. As more evidence became available "the crimes of passion" were looking more deliberate than anomalous, and in species like chimps, other females were a hazard that mothers had to watch for.

Nevertheless, chimpanzees breed so slowly that it was 1997 before Goodall and zoologist Anne Pusey had collected enough data to show a statistically significant correlation between female rank and a mother's ability to keep her infants alive. This finding caused them to reevaluate their longstanding diagnosis of Passion's "pathological" behavior. When, two decades after the first cases were reported, Fifi's daughter attacked the daughter of a subordinate female, Pusey assumed it was a failed attempt at infanticide.

Mother chimps like Flo, then, were not simply doting nurturers but entrepreneurial dynasts as well. A female's quest for status -- her ambition, if you will -- has become inseparable from her ability to keep her offspring and grand-offspring alive. Far from conflicting with maternity, such a female's "ambitious" tendencies are part and parcel of maternal success.   


 Mother Nature: A History of Mothers, Infants, and Natural Selection. Blaffer Hrdy, p. 50-52.


https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=cABykBgcRKo
 CARTA: Childrearing in Human Evolution--Melvin Konner:Hunter-Gatherer Childhood and Human Evolution

The perennial question "which sex to produce" can be mind-boggling, especially in such flexible primates as baboons and macaques, "weedy" species like humans are, readily adapting to diverse habitats. As in all well-studied Old World cercopithecine monkeys, baboons and macaque daughters inherit rank from their mothers. Because daughters remain nearby, it behooves a high-ranking mother to produce the sex that will benefit most from her own status, as well as bolster matrilineal interests by supporting kin (another form of local resource enhancement). In habitats like Amboseli, where food is scarce, high-ranking mothers do just this -- they overproduce daughters. The same pattern can also be documented for some populations of macaques. 

Year after year, mothers in the highest-ranking matrilines consistently produce significantly more daughters than sons, while low-ranking females produce few daughters and more sons. Low-ranking females not only produce few daughters, but such daughters as they do produce are more likely to die than sons born to mothers of equivalently low rank. Based on captive studies of bonnet macaques, Joan Silk showed that whereas sons who depart their natal group can leave the disadvantages of their mother's low rank behind, daughters cannot. In her study, no daughter born to a low-ranking mother managed to produce a single surviving offspring. When competition for local resources is intense, a daughter born to a high-status mother is the right sex in the right place at that time.


Recall that among the baboons Jeanna Altmann studied at Amboseli, infants have a 25-percent chance of dying during each of the first two years of life. But if a baby is a daughter born to a high-ranking mother -- the "right sex" -- the baby's survival chances go up twofold, and are higher than survival chances for a son born to a mother of the same status. Such daughters also breed sooner. On average, mothers who get the sex right contribute an extra half-grand-offspring to the next generation. Mothers at Amboseli produce no more than seven offspring in their lives, of which on average only two survive. Given how little these baboon mothers have to show for a lifetime spent producing and carrying babies, such bonuses add up.


Generation after generation, cumulative reproductive advantages mean that mothers in these matrilineal systems compete for more enduring stakes than the isolated copulations males fight over. A male who hitches his reproductive star to a successful matriline by siring a daughter in one, secures his ticket to posterity. Similarly, if a male's mate is a subordinate female, both parents benefit from son production. Lowborn sons, like poor country boys, strike out for distant opportunities, leaving natal disadvantages behind. But in some cercopithecine monkeys like macaques, there is another reason for subordinate mothers to bias toward sons. Females from dominant matrilines maliciously harass daughters born to competing mothers, sending a not so subtle message: "We may tolerate your sons for a time, but your daughters -- who will be permanent residents --are not welcome." These bullies inflict much wear and tear on low-ranking mothers, especially those carrying daughters. Silk hypothesized that such penalties imposed upon low-ranking mothers who produce daughters has led to selection on subordinate mothers to either avoid conceiving , or avoid gestating, daughters.


Mother Nature: A History of Mothers, Infants, and Natural Selection. Blaffer Hrdy, p. 333-334.

...Jeanne Altmann first showed that high and low-ranking baboon mothers at Amboseli differed in their probabilities of giving birth to a son versus a daughter, few knew what to make of it. Many found it hard to believe, because in order to understand what was going on one also had to take into account the social and ecological context in which each mother was operating, and to understand that baboon daughters born to low-ranking females were less likely to survive than sons were. Why? Studies of captive macaques with a similar social system provide one reason. Higher-ranking females in the same group harass mothers with daughters (the sex of offspring that will remain in the natal group and compete with her own daughters) but leave low-ranking mothers with sons alone. As a consequence, infant daughters suffer higher mortality than would sons born to mothers of the same low rank.


With the support of their mothers and other matrilineal kin, daughters born to high-ranking baboon females rise in the hierarchy and, in turn, pass on the advantages of their acquired rank (along with such perks as early reproductive maturity, and greater offspring survival) to daughters. The female baboon, like most social mammals, introduces her baby into the network of social relationships she has forged. Daughters who grow up surrounded by high-ranking kin give birth at an earlier age to offspring more likely to survive. Since baboon daughters inherit their rank from their mother, these social advantages are transmitted across generations as maternal effects, and the reproductive advantages accumulate through time in her matriline. But this strange bias in production of progeny only made sense in the light of variation between females.

Mother Nature: A History of Mothers, Infants, and Natural Selection. Blaffer Hrdy, p. 81-22.



MORE TO COME FROM THIS BOOK. I'LL TIE IN THE FEMALE DESIRE TO ATTAIN HIGH SOCIAL STATUS WITH THE CHIMPANZEE, FLO'S, ATTAINMENT OF HIGH SOCIAL RANK (YOUR AMBITION TO ATTAIN STATUS IS THE SAME AS FLO'S.) MONEY ON THE FLO!


I'LL ADD A FEW PASSAGES FROM MATT RIDLEY'S RED QUEEN CONCERNING THE BENEFITS OF HIGH SOCIAL STATUS. YOU'LL FIND IT ALL VERY INTERESTING. I PROMISE.



31m
I got what I need and if I want something else imma get it. Fuck what u talking about.
Fuck what you talking about!

Another DUMB Persian. Their IQs Are Just Above Arabs, But They're Still Among Some Of The Lowest In The World Partly Because Of Their Long History Of Inbreeding (Cousin-Cousin Sex). Anyhow, She's Just Another One Of Those DUMB Generation ME Broads That's Driven To Acquire Wealth And Attain Rank (Status) By Any Means Necessary, But Has NO IDEA Why She's Driven To Do This And NO IDEA What To Do Once She Gets It. Get Out There And Get It Girl! Fuck What I'm Talking About!